Psychology Bachelor - Developmental Specialization

 

Psychology Bachelor - Developmental Specialization

What can I find on this page?
On this page, you can find a summary for all the study materials you need for the developmental specialization of the Psychology bachelor's programme at the University of Amsterdam. There is a link for all the separate courses. The courses have been organized into so-called bundles, which contain all the separate literature (book chapters & articles) to make for an easy overview. As all the study materials have been created by an actual student (me!) who has followed the same courses you are following right now, you know the material is up-to-date and focused on you, as a student.

The following courses are included:

  • Adolescence: Developmental, Clinical, and School Psychology
  • Childhood: Clinical and School Psychology
  • Childhood: Developmental Psychology
  • Clinical Perspective on Today's Issues
  • Clinical Developmental & Health Psychology
  • Evidence-based Research Practice
  • Youth Interventions: Theory, Research, and Practice
  • Clinical Skills: Developmental Psychology

Why should I use this page?
You should use this page because it contains quality summaries for all the courses of the developmental specialization of the Bachelor's Progamme. I have used the summaries to study for my own exams and have passed all exams with at least an 8 (GPA 8.44). Therefore, you know the quality is good! Besides that, you can ask any question you might have about the study materials and I will try to answer them as soon as possible! Last, but certainly not least, the summaries are student-priced; only 5 euros to gain access for a whole year!

Additional information:
If you have any remaining questions after reading this (or want to comment on something), you are also always welcome to send me an e-mail. This can also be about study-related matters, providing you are a JoHo member. My e-mail is jespernicolai2000@gmail.com.

Questions on how to access the summaries?
If you have any questions on how to access the summaries, you can check the FAQ: https://www.joho.org/nl/samenvattingen-studiehulp-helpdesk-faq or you can contact JoHo: https://www.joho.org/nl/contact-met-joho-joho-centers!

Clinical Perspective on Today's Issues

Here you can find all the study materials needed for this course. In the 'lecture summary', you will find that the articles are integrated with the lecture materials.

Clinical Perspective on Today’s Issues – Full course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Clinical Perspective on Today’s Issues – Full course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Image

This bundle contains all the information needed for the for the course "Clinical Perspective on Today's Issues" given at the University of Amsterdam. It contains lecture information, information from the relevant books and all the articles. The following is included:

  • “Eagly & Wood (2013). The nature-nurture debates: 25 years of challenges in understanding the psychology of gender.” – Article summary 
  • “Hyde & Delamater (2017). Gender roles and stereotypes.” – Article summary 
  • “Petersen & Hyde (2010). A meta-analytic review of research on gender differences in sexuality, 1993 – 2007.” – Article summary
  • “Vanwesenbeeck (2009). Doing gender in sex and sex
.......read more
Clinical Perspective on Today’s Issues – Interim exam 1 (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Clinical Perspective on Today’s Issues – Interim exam 1 (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Image

This bundle contains all the information needed for the first interim exam for the course "Clinical Perspective on Today's Issues" given at the University of Amsterdam. It contains lecture information, information from the relevant books and all the articles. The following is included:

  • “Eagly & Wood (2013). The nature-nurture debates: 25 years of challenges in understanding the psychology of gender.” – Article summary 
  • “Hyde & Delamater (2017). Gender roles and stereotypes.” – Article summary 
  • “Petersen & Hyde (2010). A meta-analytic review of research on gender differences in sexuality, 1993 – 2007.” – Article summary
.......read more
Clinical Perspective on Today’s Issues – Interim exam 2 (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Clinical Perspective on Today’s Issues – Interim exam 2 (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Image

This bundle contains all the information needed for the second interim exam for the course "Clinical Perspective on Today's Issues" given at the University of Amsterdam. It contains lecture information, information from the relevant books and all the articles. The following is included:

  • “Brewin et al. (2009). Reformulating PTSD for DSM-V: Life after criterion A.” – Article summary
  • “Cacioppo et al. (2015). Loneliness: Clinical import and interventions.” – Article summary 
  • “DiTomasso, Brannen-McNulty, Ross, & Burgess (2003). Attachment styles, social skills and loneliness in young adults.” – Article summary 
  • “DSM-5 criteria. Posttraumatic stress disorder.” –
.......read more

Adolescence: Developmental, Clinical, and School Psychology

Here you can find all the study materials needed for this course. In the 'lecture summary', you will find that the articles are integrated with the lecture materials.

Evidence-based Clinical Practice – Full course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Evidence-based Clinical Practice – Full course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Image

This bundle gives a full overview of the course "Evidence-based Clinical Practice" given at the University of Amsterdam. It contains both the articles and the lectures. The following is included:

  • “Dennis et al. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders.” – Article summary
  • “Kazdin (2008). New opportunities to bridge clinical research and practice, enhance the knowledge base, and improve patient care.” - Article summary
  • “Kraemer et al. (2003). Measures of clinical significance.” - Article summary
  • “Funder et al. (2014). Improving the dependability of research in personality and social psychology: Recommendations for research
.......read more
Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents by Weisz and Kazdin (third edition) – Book summary

Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents by Weisz and Kazdin (third edition) – Book summary

Image

This bundle contains a book summary of the book Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents by Weisz and Kazdin (third edition). It contains the following chapters:

- 1, 2, 4, 12, 13, 15, 16, 17, 20, 29

Evidence-based Clinical Practice

Here you can find all the study materials needed for this course. In the 'lecture summary', you will find that the articles are integrated with the lecture materials.

Adolescence: Developmental, Clinical, and School Psychology – Lecture summary
Adolescence: Developmental, Clinical, and School Psychology – Article overview (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)
Adolescence and emerging adulthood: A cultural approach by Jeffrey Arnett and Malcolm Hughes (sixth edition) – Book summary

Clinical Developmental & Health Psychology

Here you can find all the study materials needed for this course. In the 'lecture summary', you will find that the articles are integrated with the lecture materials.

Clinical Developmental & Health Psychology – Full course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Clinical Developmental & Health Psychology – Full course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Image

This bundle contains all the information needed for the for the course "Clinical Developmental & Health Psychology" given at the University of Amsterdam. It contains lecture information, information from the relevant books and all the articles. The following is included:

  • “Del Giudice (2016). The evolutionary future of psychopathology.” – Article summary 
  • “Geeraerts et al. (2018). Individual differences in visual attention and self-regulation: A multimethod longitudinal study from infancy to toddlerhood.” – Article summary 
  • “Hunnius (2007). The early development of visual attention and its implications for social and cognitive development.” – Article summary 
  • “Li, van Vught, & Colarelli (2018). Corrigendum:
.......read more

Childhood: Clinical and School Psychology

Here you can find all the study materials needed for this course. In the 'lecture summary', you will find that the articles are integrated with the lecture materials.

Childhood: Clinical and School Psychology – Article overview (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Childhood: Clinical and School Psychology – Article overview (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Image

This bundle contains all the articles needed for the course "Childhood: Clinical and School Psychology" given at the University of Amsterdam. It contains the following articles:

  • Child and adolescent psychopathology by Wilmhurst (second edition) – Summary chapter 1 
  • Child and adolescent psychopathology by Wilmhurst (second edition) – Summary chapter 2 
  • Child and adolescent psychopathology by Wilmhurst (second edition) – Summary chapter 3 
  • Child and adolescent psychopathology by Wilmhurst (second edition) – Summary chapter 4 
  • Child and adolescent psychopathology by Wilmhurst (second edition) – Summary chapter 5 
  • Child and adolescent psychopathology by Wilmhurst
.......read more
Childhood: Clinical and School Psychology - Example Essay

Childhood: Clinical and School Psychology - Example Essay

Image

Which treatments are effective for ADHD?

Elementary school teachers in the Netherlands are increasingly experiencing burnout symptoms (DUO Onderwijsonderzoek, 2016). This makes it likely that the quality of education will suffer, with teachers mainly pointing out that having too many students in a classroom in need of additional assistance due to some form of psychopathology is increasing the workload (DUO Onderwijsonderzoek, 2016). One fairly common issue – at three to seven percent of the children in a classroom – is the presence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a neurodevelopmental disorder in which a child mainly presents with problems controlling impulses, remaining concentrated on the task at hand, and hyperactivity (Wilmhurst, 2015; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2019). Next to making the already challenging task of an elementary teacher more straining, the disorder is associated with a range of negative outcomes, such as the presence of comorbid disorders, family problems and substance abuse (DUO Onderwijsonderzoek, 2016; Wilmhurst, 2015). To minimize these adverse effects and alleviate some of the workload of educators, it is essential to tackle this disorder (DUO Onderwijsonderzoek, 2016; Wilmhurst, 2015; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2019).

ADHD is a persistent and heterogeneous disorder that consists of several subtypes (Wilmhurst, 2015). While it may be useful to tailor interventions to these subtypes (Barnard, Stevens, To, Lan, & Mulsow, 2009; Abikoff, 2001), the best results in treating ADHD may be obtained by targeting the theorized causes of ADHD as knowledge of the mechanisms of change allows for optimization of an intervention (Moldovan & Pintean, 2015; Kangaslampi & Peltonen, 2019). According to the dual pathway model of ADHD (Sonuga-Barke, 2002), the disorder consists of deficits in executive function and impairments in the motivational and reward system. The former pathway is reflected in both symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity (Franke et al., 2018; Dovis, van der Oord, Wiers, & Prins, 2012) and inattention (Franke et al., 2018) while the latter pathway is mainly associated with hyperactive and impulsive symptoms (Boyer, 2021). Following this model, it may be that interventions tailored to either of these pathways are useful in ameliorating ADHD symptoms.

Steiner, Frenette, Rene, Brennan, and Perin (2014) compared two interventions – both of which are aimed at reducing ADHD symptoms by improving executive functioning – with each other and a control group. The definition of executive function is controversial but most definitions include, among other things, working memory, shifting and inhibition (Zelazo, & Müller, 2011). The first treatment, cognitive feedback, uses feedback to reinforce correct responses on a task and this is believed to improve attention, impulsivity and working memory (Klingberg et al., 2005; Rabiner, Murray, Skinner, & Malone, 2009). While this mainly focuses on the response of the child, the second intervention, neurofeedback, focuses feedback on a person’s attention level. Direct auditory and visual feedback allows for better monitoring of one’s situation and this is believed to improve executive function through changing one’s brainwave patterns (Heinrich, Gevensleben, & Strehl, 2007). Both treatments showed significant improvements on measures of ADHD compared to the control and the results were maintained over six months. Surprisingly, the participants in the neurofeedback condition scored better than those in the cognitive feedback group, even though both treatments are believed to improve executive functions (Klingberg et al., 2005; Rabiner et al., 2009; Heinrich et al., 2007). Of particular interest is that the use of stimulant medication increased in the cognitive feedback and control group. While usage is understandable, as this is a common treatment for ADHD (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020; Daley et al., 2014), it may have confounded the results since medication is believed to have a direct effect on symptoms (Daley et al., 2014). Participants did not increase their stimulant medication use in the neurofeedback condition while showing the greatest improvements on measures of ADHD. This implies that medication may negatively impact the effectiveness of behavioural interventions. Alternatively, the interaction between medication and the treatment may be essential, meaning that the change in dose could have led to the change, rather than medication use itself. Indeed, research has shown that stimulant medication may enable treatments to become effective (Franke et al., 2018). While Steiner et al. (2014) demonstrated that ADHD symptoms can be reduced by improving executive function, it may be useful to scrutinize the second pathway of the dual pathway model (Sonuga-Barke, 2002), as the interaction between stimulant medication use and executive function improvement cannot be excluded.

Children with ADHD may underperform tasks due to executive function deficits but they often also appear to be unmotivated (Sonuga-Barke, 2002; Dovis et al., 2011) and rarely show task persistence (Dovis et al., 2011). Nonetheless, parents report that this is not the case for some activities (Boyer, 2021), such as videogames (Carlson, Booth, Shin, & Canu, 2002; Boyer, 2021). This implies that interventions aimed at enhancing motivation may be successful in improving task performance and decreasing ADHD symptoms. To further analyze this matter, Dovis et al. (2011) investigated the role of reinforcement on task performance. They differentiated between four types of reinforcement; feedback, low reward, high reward, and computer-based reward. While – for children with ADHD– every form of incentive improved performance compared to only receiving feedback, performance did not reach the level of controls. Computer-based rewards and higher monetary rewards did not differ,  and both outperformed smaller incentives. It is unrealistic to continuously provide the child with financial incentives, thus video games may be a useful method of improving task performance and persistence. The effectiveness of using technology-based rewards may be explained by the reinforcement pattern. Given that a child with ADHD is very sensitive to rewards (Dovis et al., 2011; Sonuga-Barke, 2002; Boyer, 2021) and the reward pattern, as is the case with videogames, is continuous and immediate (Sault, 2015), this may have explained the improved task performance. Similarly, task persistence may be enhanced by employing a comparable pattern as the child with ADHD is easily distracted (Wilmhurst, 2015) and invariably looking for new rewards (Dovis et al., 2011; Boyer, 2021). Indeed, Dovis et al. (2011) found that by utilizing this method, task persistence improved and normalized with the highest incentives and the computer-based intervention. While the relationship between reinforcement and stimulant medication has not fully been explored yet (Dovis et al., 2011), it may have an additive effect on interventions aimed at the motivation and reward system since stimulant medication increases dopamine levels in the brain (Zuvekas & Vitiello, 2012; Heal, Smith, Gosden, & Nutt, 2013) and the reward system is a dopaminergic system (Arrias-Carrión, Stamelou, Murillo-Rodríguez, Menédz-Gonzáles, & Pöppel, 2010; Volkow et al., 2010). Although providing reinforcement may not directly treat ADHD, as the symptoms may recur when incentives are removed (Dovis et al., 2011), motivation may be an essential aspect of interventions aimed at ADHD. Enhancing motivation may optimize treatments and could allow for better management of the disorder.

By aiming treatment at improving executive functioning, it is possible to obtain both a reduction in ADHD symptoms and achieve a lasting change (Steiner et al., 2014). Furthermore, treatment aimed at increasing motivation allows for improving task performance and persistence (Dovis et al., 2011). Thus, by tackling the theorized underlying mechanisms of ADHD, it is possible to alleviate some of the adverse effects of the disorder (Sonagu-Barke, 2002; Steiner et al., 2014; Dovis et al., 2011). In addition to this, it can allow for optimalization of present and future treatments (Moldovan & Pintean, 2015; Kangaslampi & Peltonen, 2019) such as by making interventions more engaging through the use of videogames (Dovis et al., 2011; Boyer, 2021; Sault, 2015). Albeit the relationship between behavioural interventions aimed at improving the hypothesized causal mechanisms of ADHD with stimulant medication has not been fully explored, it is an effective method of decreasing symptoms (Franke et al., 2018; Daley et al., 2014; Arnold et al., 2003; Dovis et al., 2011). The best outcomes are obtained when the child is able to choose the treatment of preference from the vast number of treatments targeting different developmental pathways of ADHD (Boyer et al., 2016; Dovis et al., 2011; Frenette et al., 2014). This may be due to the heterogeneity of the disorder (Wilmhurst, 2015; Boyer et al., 2016). To conclude, there is no ‘perfect’ treatment for a heterogeneous disorder such as ADHD (Wilmhurst, 2015; Dovis et al., 2011; Frenette et al., 2014). It is important to personalize treatment (Abikoff, 2011) and, importantly, listen to the one who it is all about; the child with ADHD (Boyer et al., 2016). By doing this, the negative outcomes of ADHD can be reduced and some of the workload of elementary school teachers can be alleviated, leading to higher quality education.

 

 

Reference list

Abikoff, H. (2001). Tailored psychosocial treatments for ADHD: The search for a good fit. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 30(1), 122–125. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3001_14

Arias-Carrión, O., Stamelou, M., Murillo-Rodríguez, E., Menédez-Gonzáles, M., & Pöppel, E. (2010). Dopaminergic reward system: A short integrative review. Int Arch Med, 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1186/1755-7682-3-24

Arnold, L. E., Elliott, M., Sachs, L., Bird, H., Kraemer, H. C., Wells, K. C., … Wigal, T. (2003). Effects of ethnicity on treatment attendance, stimulant response/dose, and 14-month outcome in ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(4), 713–727. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.71.4.713

Barnard, L., Stevens, T., To, Y. M., Lan, W. Y., & Mulsow, M. (2009). The importance of ADHD subtype classification for educational applications of DSM-V. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(6), 573–583. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054708326433

Boyer, B. (2021, February 17). ADHD and behavioral problems [Slides]. Retrieved from https://canvas.uva.nl/courses/21056/pages/lecture-3-adhd-and-behavioral-problems-slides-and-audio?module_item_id=846889

Boyer, B. E., Doove, L. L., Geurts, H. M., Prins, P. J. M., Van Mechelen, I., & Van der Oord, S. (2016). Qualitative treatment-subgroup interactions in a randomized clinical trial of treatments for adolescents with ADHD: Exploring what cognitive-behavioral treatment works for whom. PLOS ONE, 11(3), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150698

Carlson, C. L., Booth, J. E., Shin, M., & Canu, W. H. (2002). Parent-, teacher-, and self-rated motivational styles in ADHD subtypes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(2), 104–113. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940203500202

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2019, November 7). ADHD in the classroom | CDC. Retrieved 24 February 2021, from https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/adhd/school-success.html

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020, September 21). Treatment of ADHD | CDC. Retrieved 26 February 2021, from https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/adhd/treatment.html

Daley, D., van der Oord, S., Ferrin, M., Danckaerts, M., Doepfner, M., Cortese, S., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2014). Behavioral interventions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials across multiple outcome domains. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(8), 835–847. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.013

Dekkers, T. (2020, September 11). Development of ADHD [Slides]. Retrieved from https://canvas.uva.nl/courses/18583/pages/september-11-development-of-adhd?module_item_id=635199

Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R. W., & Prins, P. J. M. (2011). Can motivation normalize working memory and task persistence in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder? The effects of money and Computer-Gaming. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(5), 669–681. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9601-8

DUO Onderwijsonderzoek. (2016, January). Rapportage werkdruk leerkrachten in het basisonderwijs. Retrieved from https://www.duo-onderwijsonderzoek.nl/wp-content/uploads/2016/01/Rapportage-Werkdruk-Leerkrachten-PO-8-januari-2016-1.pdf

Franke, B., Michelini, G., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Bilbow, A., Buitelaar, J. K., … Reif, A. (2018). Live fast, die young? A review on the developmental trajectories of ADHD across the lifespan. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 28(10), 1059–1088. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.08.001

Heal, D. J., Smith, S. L., Gosden, J., & Nutt, D. J. (2013). Amphetamine, past and present – a pharmacological and clinical perspective. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 27(6), 479–496. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881113482532

Heinrich, H., Gevensleben, H., & Strehl, U. (2007). Annotation: Neurofeedback - train your brain to train behaviour. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(1), 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01665.x

Kangaslampi, S., & Peltonen, K. (2019). Mechanisms of change in psychological interventions for posttraumatic stress symptoms: A systematic review with recommendations. Current Psychology, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00478-5

Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., … Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD- a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177–186. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010

Moldovan, R., & Pintea, S. (2015). Mechanisms of change in psychotherapy: Methodological and statistical considerations. Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 299–311. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/296431413_Mechanisms_of_change_in_psychotherapy_Methodological_and_statistical_considerations

Rabiner, D. L., Murray, D. W., Skinner, A. T., & Malone, P. S. (2009). A randomized trial of two promising computer-based interventions for students with attention difficulties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(1), 131–142. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9353-x

Sault, D. M. (2015, October). The influence of videogame reinforcement schedules on game play duration. Retrieved from https://eprints.utas.edu.au/23558/1/Sault_whole_thesis.pdf

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioural Brain Research, 130(1–2), 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00432-6

Steiner, N. J., Frenette, E. C., Rene, K. M., Brennan, R. T., & Perrin, E. C. (2014). In-school neurofeedback training for ADHD: Sustained improvements from a randomized control trial. Pediatrics, 133(3), 483–492. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-2059

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Newcorn, J. H., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Telang, F., … Swanson, J. M. (2010). Motivation deficit in ADHD is associated with dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Molecular Psychiatry, 16(11), 1147–1154. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2010.97

Wilmshurst, L. (2015). Essentials of child and adolescent psychopathology (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ, United States: Wiley.

Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.). The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, Second Edition, 574–603. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444325485

Zuvekas, S. H., & Vitiello, B. (2012). Stimulant medication use in children: A 12-year perspective. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(2), 160–166. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11030387

Youth Interventions: Theory, Research, and Practice

Here you can find all the study materials needed for this course. In the 'lecture summary', you will find that the articles are integrated with the lecture materials.

Youth Interventions: Theory, Research, and Practice – Lecture summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)
Youth Interventions: Theory, Research, and Practice – Article overview (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Childhood: Developmental Psychology & Clinical Skills: Developmental Psychology

Here you can find all the study materials needed for this course. In the 'lecture summary', you will find that the articles are integrated with the lecture materials.

Childhood: Developmental Psychology – Lecture summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)
Childhood: Developmental Psychology – Article overview (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Childhood: Developmental Psychology – Article overview (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Image

This bundle contains a summary of all the articles that are needed for the course "Childhood: Developmental Psychology" given at the University of Amsterdam. It includes the following articles:

  • Bjorklund & Causey (2017). Biological bases of development” – Article summary 
  • “Kuppens & Ceulemans (2019). Parenting styles: A closer look at a well-known concept”. – Article summary 
  • “Taraban & Shaw (2018). Parenting in context: Revisiting Belsky’s classic process of parenting model in early childhood.” – Article summary 
  • “Tucker-Drob, Briley, & Harden (2013). Genetic and environmental influences on cognition across development and context.” – Article summary 
.......read more
Childhood: Developmental Psychology - Example Essay

Childhood: Developmental Psychology - Example Essay

Image

Does age of migration matter for how well a child can learn a second language?

In the past 50 years, the number of migrants worldwide has been steadily increasing (United Nations, 2019a, pp. 21) with 82 million in Europe alone in 2019 (United Nations, 2019b). It is crucial to note that about a sixth of those are younger than 20 years old (United Nations, 2019a) as age appears to problematically impact the consequences of migration. To illustrate, migrant youth is more likely to get into contact with the law (van der Gaag, 2019), suffer from substance abuse (Mechlor-Ayala, 2019) and be victimized by their peers (Heine, 2008) which negatively influence health outcomes, such as loneliness (Matthews et al., 2020). This does not mean that people who migrate are doomed to experience adverse effects for the rest of their life. Successful integration diminishes the negative effects experienced as a result of migration (Heine, 2008) and being younger makes it easier to successfully integrate into a new country (Heine, 2008). Thus, although age may be a risk factor in migration, it may also be a protective factor. One aspect which is essential for a successful acculturation process is learning the language of the host country (Heine, 2008). Therefore, the current essay will scrutinize whether the age of migration matters for how well a child can learn a second language.

Age appears to influence the ease of acquisition of language (e.g., Johnson & Newport, 1989). One of the most appalling pieces of evidence for this comes from the case of Genie, a 13-year-old female who spent most of her life in isolation and circumstances of extreme deprivation (Fromkin, Krashen, Curtiss, Rigler, & Rigler, 1974). While she was able to acquire a great deal of vocabulary, she had severe difficulties with syntactic aspects of the language, such as simple grammar rules (Fromkin et al., 1974). Although this suggests the existence of a critical period of language learning, there are significant debates about her overall cognitive ability (Fromkin et al., 1974; Curtiss, 1977; Rymer, 1993), which makes drawing conclusions about language acquisition problematic. Other research since then has found that the first year of life is essential for the development of syntax (Friedmann & Rusou, 2015) and that early life experience with language is indispensable for language development in general (Siegler, 2006). This means that people who migrate relatively early in life will, most likely, not have any difficulties in acquiring the language of the host country as this will either become the native language or the child will develop bilingualism (Bialystok, 2018; Heine, 2008). However, in the case of migrants, the language of the host country will most likely be the second language rather than the first (United Nations, 2019a). This implies that, while age appears to influence the ease of first language acquisition, the same pattern does not need to hold for a second language. Hartshorne, Tenenbaum, and Pinker (2018) used more than 600.000 participants to investigate the learning rate of a second language for non-native English speakers. By employing a complex mathematical model, they found that people’s learning rate for a language tends to decrease with age, starting at age 17. Younger migrants thus have more ease in learning a second language. Though some research has shown that the level of ultimate attainment decreases after adolescence (Johnson & Newport, 1989), Hartshorne et al. (2018) demonstrate that this need not be true as only the ease of acquisition changes with age. Factors other than age may influence the ultimate level of attainment as age is not the sole determinant of this. Given that people who learn a second language before adulthood often do have a greater proficiency (Johnson & Newport, 1989; Hartshorne et al., 2018), it may be that these factors interact with age.

One of these factors which is especially relevant for migrants is difficulties in the acculturation process. This could influence one’s ultimate level of proficiency in a second language. Being part of a minority, as migrants often are, puts one at risk of being victimized and, ironically, this obstructs the pathway to successful integration (Heine, 2008). Matthews et al. (2020) found that being victimized in the form of bullying in childhood while observing other peer relationships as unproblematic and normal leads to a sense of injustice and the perception of being an outsider. This leads to increased feelings of loneliness (Matthews et al., 2020). The feeling of being alienated from the host country could have a direct effect on one’s ultimate language proficiency as these people may minimize their attempts to engage with the host culture, leading to reduced opportunities to practice and learn a new language (Heine, 2008). This is exemplified by Barry’s model of acculturation (Berry, 1997) which states that acculturation strategies that minimize engaging with the host culture lead to negative migrant outcomes, including language learning. Loneliness could also have an indirect effect on language learning as it negatively influences one’s academic achievements (Lin & Huang, 2012). Migrants that have been bullied during development may have more problems in acquiring a second language. However, bullying in childhood may only lead to adult loneliness if one has a genetic predisposition to feel alone (Matthews et al., 2020). Children who are integrated more get victimized less often and integration is easier at a young age (Heine, 2008). This implies that people who migrate before the age of formal schooling will experience more ease in language acquisition as they will be integrated more (Heine, 2008). Although peer victimization is more common for migrants (Eurostat, 2019; Heine, 2008), as Matthews et al. (2020) rightly point out, bullying is not the only form of peer victimization and it is unlikely that every migrant child or adolescent who does not achieve a native level of language proficiency has been bullied in the past and has a genetic disposition to feel lonely. This means that other difficulties in the acculturation process may influence a migrant’s ultimate level of proficiency. For example, though the main reason for migration is seeking economic betterment (United Nations, 2019a), many migrants are at risk of monetary poverty. This low socioeconomic status makes it unsurprising that they live in impoverished areas more often (Eurostat, 2021) and this could complicate the acculturation process (Gracia et al., 2012; Heine, 2008). Gracia et al. (2012) found that experiencing more neighbourhood violence during childhood or adolescence is associated with poor academic achievement or problematic behaviour, which may negatively impact language acquisition (Hartshorne, 2018). Though this likely impacts all age categories, children and adolescents may be more sensitive to a negative living environment due to their stronger connections to the area as a result of having multiple playmates (Gracia et al., 2012). This means that age may moderate the effects of neighbourhood violence on developmental outcomes (Gracia et al., 2012) and this may negatively influence language proficiency (Hartshorne, 2018).

To conclude, the age at which somebody migrates matters for how well a child can learn a second language. Children and adolescents experience fewer difficulties when it comes to learning a second language compared to adults (Johnson & Newport, 1989; Hartshorne, 2018). Since successful acquisition of the language of the host country is essential for the acculturation process (Heine, 2008), age can be a protective factor for migrant children. However, migrants are at increased risk of experiencing negative life events, such as being victimized by peers (Eurostat, 2021; Heine, 2008) or perceiving neighbourhood violence (Gracia et al., 2012) and this may make learning a second language more problematic (Lin & Huang, 2012; Gracia et al., 2021; Matthews et al., 2020). Fortunately, most people who arrive in a new host country before the age of 16 become very proficient in the language of the host country (Johnson & Newport, 1989) though negative life events could make this process more problematic (Matthews et al., 2020; Gracia et al., 2012). While this does not mean that people who arrive after this period will never be able to become proficient in the language of the host country (Hartshorne, 2018), they will have more difficulties with this (Johnson & Newport, 1989; Gracia et al., 2012; Hartshorne, 2018; Matthews et al., 2020; Lin & Huang, 2012). It may be impossible to alleviate all the stressors associated with the acculturation process but it could be facilitated by aiding migrants in acquiring the language of the host country (Heine, 2008) by, for example, providing them with compulsory training. While future studies need to identify more factors that influence language learning for migrants, research does show that in the elegant complexity of language acquisition, age is both a protective and a risk factor (Matthews et al., 2020; Gracia et al., 2012; Lin & Huang, 2012; Johnson & Newport, 1989; Hartshorne, 2018).

 

 

Reference list

Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46(1), 5–68. Retrieved from https://iaap-journals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01087.x

Bialystok, E. (2018). Bilingual education for young children: Review of the effects and consequences. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21(6), 666–679. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1203859

Curtiss, S. (1977). Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern-day wild child. Boston, Massachusetts: Academic Press.

Eurostat. (2021, January). Migrant integration statistics - at risk of poverty and social exclusion - statistics explained. Retrieved 23 February 2021, from https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/Migrant_integration_statistics_-_at_risk_of_poverty_and_social_exclusion

Friedmann, N., & Rusou, D. (2015). Critical period for first language: The crucial role of language input during the first year of life. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 35, 27–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.003

Fromkin, V., Krashen, S., Curtiss, S., Rigler, D., & Rigler, M. (1974). The development of language in Genie: A case of language acquisition beyond the ‘critical period’. Brain and Language, 1, 81–107. Retrieved from https://linguistics.ucla.edu/people/curtiss/1974%20-%20The%20development%20of%20language%20in%20Genie.pdf

Gracia, E., Fuentes, M. C., Garcia, F., & Lila, M. (2012). Perceived neighborhood violence, parenting styles, and developmental outcomes among Spanish adolescents. Journal of Community Psychology, 40(8), 1004–1021. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.21512

Hartshorne, J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 177, 263–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.007

Heine, S. J. (2008). Cultural psychology (International Student Edition, Vol. 3). New York, United States of America: W.W. Norton.

Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 60–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0

Lin, S.-H., & Huang, Y.-C. (2012). Investigating the relationships between loneliness and learning burnout. Active Learning in Higher Education, 13(3), 231–243. https://doi.org/10.1177/1469787412452983

Matthews, T., Caspi, A., Danese, A., Fisher, H. L., Moffitt, T. E., & Arseneault, L. (2020). A longitudinal twin study of victimization and loneliness from childhood to young adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579420001005

Melchor-Ayala, O. (2019). The influence of age at migration on criminal offending among Foreign-Born immigrants. Dissertations and Theses, 5179, 1–136. https://doi.org/10.15760/etd.7055

Rymer, R. (1993). Genie: A scientific tragedy. New York, United States: Macmillan Publishers.

Siegler, R. S. (2006). How children develop, exploring child develop student media tool kit & scientific american reader to accompany how children develop (Second edition). New York, United States of America: Worth Publishers.

United Nations. (2019a). World migration report 2020. Geneva, Switzerland: International Organization for Migration.

United Nations. (2019b, September 20). Number of migrants now growing faster than world population, new UN. Retrieved 23 February 2021, from https://news.un.org/en/story/2019/09/1046562

van der Gaag, R. S. (2019). The crime–immigration nexus: Cultural alignment and structural influences in self-reported serious youth delinquent offending among migrant and native youth. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 35(4), 431–460. https://doi.org/10.1177/1043986219881827

“Clinical Skills: Developmental Psychology – Course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)"

“Clinical Skills: Developmental Psychology – Course summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)"

Image

This bundle contains everything you need to know for the course "Clinical Skills: Developmental Psychology" given at the University of Amsterdam. It contains all the lectures and the following chapters of the books:

Clinical assessment of child and adolescent personality and behaviour by Frick, Barry, & Kamphaus (fourth edition): 2, 3, 4, 5, 13, 15, 16
Psychological communication: Theories, roles and skills for counsellors by van der Molen, Lang, Trower, & Look (second edition) – 2, 5, 6, 7

Comments, Compliments & Kudos

Summary materials

Jesper, thanks a lot for the clear summary of the study material for the developmental specialization of the Psychology bachelor's program at the University of Amsterdam. It will certainly help students find their needed summary!

Collection

What a great collection of all materials! Really useful for anyone with this subject 

Thanks for the Summaries :D

Hey thanks a lot for all the summaries they are super helpful :D. Just one question are the parts of the chapter in the book also included in the Lectures summaries of CDP and CDSP? Thanks for your help :) 

Reply to Sebi

Hi Sebi, thanks for your kind words! To answer your question, the lecture summaries are an integrated summary of both the lecture and the literature you needed to read (so either the book, articles or both!). The main focus is on the lecture information, but everything is included!

Reply to Sebi

Hi Sebi, thanks for your kind words! To answer your question, the lecture summaries are an integrated summary of both the lecture and the literature you needed to read (so either the book, articles or both!). The main focus is on the lecture information, but everything is included!

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.
Follow the author: JesperN
Image
Access level of this page
  • Public
  • WorldSupporters only
  • JoHo members
  • Private
Statistics
[totalcount] 5 4 4
Last updated
17-02-2022
Search