How is fallacy formed in you mind? - Chapter 15

A famous and controversial experiment is known as the ‘Linda problem’. It was made up by Amos and Kahneman to demonstrate the role of heuristics in judgment and their incompatibility with logic. People who read the description of Linda instantly knew what University she had attended, because it was known for its politically engaged, radical students. Participants were asked to read a list of possible scenarios regarding Linda and rank them by representativeness and by probability. They agreed that one scenario (“She is a feminist bank teller”) seems more likely than another one (“She is a bank teller”). The stereotype of bank tellers does not include feminism, but adding that part to the description results into a more coherent story. The twist is found in the judgments of probability, because there is a logical connection between both scenarios. Since every feminist bank teller is a bank teller, the probability of Linda being a feminist bank teller must be lower than the probability of Linda being merely a bank teller. Specifying possible events in greater detail always lowers the probability. This issue causes a conflict between the logic of probability and the intuition of representativeness. The participants ranked ‘feminist bank teller’ higher than ‘bank teller’ in their ranking by probability and by resemblance.

The scenarios ‘bank teller’ and ‘feminist bank teller’ were placed on the list as number 6 and 8, close to each other. Kahneman and Amos expected that participants would notice the connection between them and that their rankings would follow logic. But surprisingly, they had ranked ‘feminist bank teller’ as more likely. Representativeness had won the battle, which is considered a failure of System 2. Nearly 90% of the participants violated the logical rule. Failing to apply an obviously relevant logical rule is called ‘fallacy’. It is called a ‘conjunction fallacy’ when people judge a conjunction of two events (in this case: feminist and bank teller) to be more probable than one of the events (bank teller) in a straight comparison.

In the short version of the Linda problem, participants had to answer the question which of the following alternatives is more likely: “She is a bank teller” or “She is a bank teller and a feminist”. Most of them gave the right answer: “bank teller”. The difference with the long version is the separation between these outcomes by the intervening seventh scenario on the list: they were judged independently, no comparison was made between them. The short version involved a direct comparison, which mobilized System 2 and prevented the commitment of the fallacy.

Hsee’s dinnerware study demonstrated how absurd the less-is-more pattern is. He asked participants to price dinnerware sets. One group was shown a display that allowed a comparison between two sets (set X: 40 pieces of which 9 are broken and set Z: 24 pieces). This is called a ‘joint evaluation’. The two other groups were shown only one set, making it a ‘single evaluation’. Which set is worth more? Set X contains the same pieces as set Z and seven extra pieces, so it must be valued higher. The participants in the joint evaluation group priced set X higher, but the participants in the single evaluation group valued set Z much higher. This happened because the average value of the pieces is much lower for set X due to the broken pieces and the single evaluation was dominated by the average. Hsee calls this pattern ‘less is more’. Removing broken pieces from set X improves the value, just like adding a high valued item increases the value of the set.

The incidence of the conjunction fallacy can be reduced by formulating an easier question. The question “What percentage of the participants…?” is much harder than “How many of the 100 participants..?” 100 people are easier to imagine, while the percentage-question does not make you think of individuals.

The experiments learn us that System 2 is not very alert and lazy.

Join World Supporter
Join World Supporter
Log in or create your free account

Why create an account?

  • Your WorldSupporter account gives you access to all functionalities of the platform
  • Once you are logged in, you can:
    • Save pages to your favorites
    • Give feedback or share contributions
    • participate in discussions
    • share your own contributions through the 7 WorldSupporter tools
Follow the author: Psychology Supporter
Content categories
Comments, Compliments & Kudos

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.
Promotions
Image
The JoHo Insurances Foundation is specialized in insurances for travel, work, study, volunteer, internships an long stay abroad
Check the options on joho.org (international insurances) or go direct to JoHo's https://www.expatinsurances.org

 

WorldSupporter Resources
Summary per chapter with the 1st edition of Thinking, Fast and Slow by Kahneman

Summary per chapter with the 1st edition of Thinking, Fast and Slow by Kahneman

Summary per chapter with the 1st edition of Thinking, Fast and Slow by Kahneman

  • What is the book about?
  • Part 1: How do fast thinking and slow thinking work? Chapters 1-9
  • Part 2: How do heuristics and biases work? Chapters 10-18
  • Part 3: In what ways can you get overconfident? Chapters 19-24
  • Part 4: How do you make choices and decisions? Chapters 25-34
  • Part 5: What is the effect of fast and slow thinking on your experiences, choices and well-being? Chapters 35-38
  • Related summaries and study notes with the 1st edition of Thinking,
.......read more